Kiliwa Indians
The Kiliwa Indians are a Mexican branch of the Upland Yuman tribes that lived in Baja California.
Historically they occupied a territory lying between the Cochimí on the south and the Paipai on the north, and extending from San Felipe on the Gulf of California to San Quintín on the Pacific coast.
Their traditional language is the Kiliwa language.
The Ñakipa have sometimes been distinguished from the Kiliwa as a separate ethnolinguistic group within the southwestern portion of what is considered Kiliwa territory. The limited linguistic evidence that is available for the Ñakipa indicates that they spoke the same language as the Eastern Kiliwa.
Radiocarbon dates and Clovis points from farther south on the peninsula suggest that the initial occupation to the north must have occurred prior to 11,000 years ago.
Kiliwa History
The Kiliwa first encountered Europeans when Juan Rodríguez Cabrillo reached the San Quintín area in 1542. There were few subsequent contacts during the next two centuries.
The Jesuit missionary-explorer Wenceslaus Linck came overland from the south into the eastern part of Kiliwa territory in 1766. The expedition to establish Spanish settlements in California, led by Gaspar de Portolà and Junípero Serra passed through the western portions.
The Dominican mission of Santo Domingo was founded in Kiliwa territory near the coast in 1775. It was followed by an inland mission of San Pedro Mártir in 1794. By around the time of Mexican independence in 1821, the population at the Kiliwa missions had sharply declined.
In 1929, Meigs reported that only 36 adult Kiliwa were then living, primarily at three settlements around Arroyo León, at San Isidoro, and in Valle Trinidad.
Twenty years later in 1949, Hohenthal found 30 adult Kiliwa living at four settlements, including Arroyo León, Agua Caliente, La Parra, and Tepí.
Meigs suggested that the original aboriginal population of the Kiliwa was about 1,300 individuals, or a density of about 0.3 persons per square kilometer. He excluded the southwestern “Ñakipa” territory from his estimates, which would raise the total to at least 2,000. Meigs considered his estimate to be a “conservative” one.
However, Roger C. Owen has argued that Meigs’ population estimates were substantially too high.
Kiliwa Culture
Information about the cultural practices of the pre-contact Kiliwa comes from a variety of sources.
These include accounts of early explorers, such as Juan Rodríguez Cabrillo and Sebastián Vizcaíno; from late eighteenth and early nineteenth century observers, such as Luis Sales and José Longinos Martínez; and from twentieth century ethnographers, including Peveril Meigs, William D. Hohenthal, and Jesús Ángel Ochoa Zazueta.
Aboriginal Kiliwa subsistence was based on hunting and gathering of natural animal and plants rather than on agriculture.
At least two dozen different plants were food resources, and many others were used for medicine or as materials for construction or craft products.
Pit-roasted Agave (mescal; ječà) was the most important plant food. In the fall season, harvesting of acorns and pine nuts from the higher-elevation portions of Kiliwa territory was a major activity.
Rabbits and deer were the most important animal food sources, but a wide range of others were also hunted, including pronghorn antelope, bighorn sheep, medium-sized mammals such as mountain lions, many small mammal species, birds, reptiles, fish, and shellfish.
Jackrabbits and quail were hunted communally, by being driven into nets. Treks were made to San Felipe on the east coast to harvest fish and shellfish and to collect salt.
Crop growing and stock raising were introduced during the historic period. Another highly valued food resource that was introduced during the historic period was wild honey.
The traditional material culture of the Kiliwa was not highly elaborate, as would be expected for a seasonally mobile group.
Structures included semi-subterranean houses with willow poles and thatching, ramadas, and sweat houses.
Hunting equipment included willow bows; arrows with carrizo reed shafts, wooden foreshafts, and stone points; wooden throwing sticks; and agave fiber nets.
Processing equipment included manos and metates for grinding seeds, wooden drills and hearths for making fire, and pottery and basketry for cooking, storing, and transporting.
Clothing was generally lacking for men. Women wore deer hide aprons and reed caps. Both sexes wore agave fiber sandals. Rabbitskin blankets provided warmth, and human hair capes were used in ceremonies. Cradles were used to transport infants.
The Kiliwa creation myth conspicuously diverges from those of the other Yuman-speaking groups. However, the Kiliwa name for the creator, Metipá, recalls similar figures in other Yuman myths.
Kiliwa Social organization
Traditional leadership roles in communities and kin groups were held on a hereditary basis, but subject to an assessment of the individual leader’s competence. Leader’s authority does not seem to have been extensive.
Kinship and community membership seems to have been defined to a large extent on the basis of patrilineal inheritance. Two levels of patrilineages (or clans, sibs) were recognized, corresponding to the šimułs of other western Yuman groups.
Maselkwa were the smaller, more strongly localized groups. Several maselkwa might collectively constitute an ichiupu.
On the broadest level, all Kiliwa were believed to be descended from four mythic brothers, the sons of the Creator.
Social recreations included a variety of games: racing with balls, shinny, top spinning, archery, dice, a guessing game, and, most importantly, peón.
Music was produced by singing and by instruments, including flutes, rattles, clappers, and bullroarers.
Kiliwa Ceremonies
Shamans were believed to be able to effect magical cures of disease or injuries, or to cause them. They presided at some religious ceremonies, and they were thought to transform themselves into animals or birds and to bring rain.
Most documented Kiliwa ceremonies were linked to rites of passage in the lives of individuals:
Birth involved various taboos or special requirements, but it was primarily a private family matter. The father observed a couvade.
Boys’ initiation for groups of boys aged around 15 years was marked by the nose-piercing ceremony (mipípŭsá) and other activities extending over two months.
Girls’ initiation at the time of first menstruation involved being baked in ashes for five days, washed in cold water for five days, and made to observe various prescriptions and taboos.
Marriage involved little ceremony, other than the groom giving gifts to the bride’s parents. Marrying up to two wives was permitted, and divorce was easy for either party.
Death was the focus of the most elaborate Kiliwa ceremonies, occurring both immediately after an individual’s death and as subsequent collective mourning observances. Talking to the dead and burning his possessions are among the formal actions.